Submit a report

Announcements

Please note that we will be CLOSED to ALL SUBMISSIONS from 1 December 2024 through 12 January 2025 to give our recommenders and reviewers a holiday break.

We are recruiting recommenders (editors) from all research fields!

Your feedback matters! If you have authored or reviewed a Registered Report at Peer Community in Registered Reports, then please take 5 minutes to leave anonymous feedback about your experience, and view community ratings.

849

When children can explain why they believe a claim, they suggest better empirical tests for those claimsuse asterix (*) to get italics
Tone K. Hermansen, Kamilla F. Mathisen, Samuel RonfardPlease use the format "First name initials family name" as in "Marie S. Curie, Niels H. D. Bohr, Albert Einstein, John R. R. Tolkien, Donna T. Strickland"
2024
<p>Hearing about surprising phenomena triggers exploration, even in young children. This exploration increases and changes with age. It becomes more targeted and efficient with children around 6-years-old clearly exploring with the intent to verify what they have been told. What underlies this development? In this study, we tested the hypothesis that children’s ability to reflect on the causes of their uncertainty about a surprising claim allows them to better target their empirical investigation of that claim—and that this ability increases with age. To test this developmental account, we assigned 4-7-year-old children (N = 174, Mage = 68.77 months, Range: 48-84 months, 52.87% girls, 47.13% boys) to one of two conditions: a prompted and an unprompted condition. In each condition, children witnessed a series of vignettes where an adult presented them with a surprising claim about an object. Children were then asked whether they thought the claim was true or not, and how certain or uncertain they were in that belief. Then, in the prompted condition, children were asked why they felt that way. Finally, in both conditions, children were asked to recommend a course of action to determine whether the adult’s claim was true or not. As predicted, the findings from this study, revealed that older children are more likely to justify their beliefs than younger children, and are also more likely to suggest targeted empirical tests for a claim—tests that provide the needed evidence to confirm or disconfirm the truth of a claim. However, although our confirmatory analyses showed that prompting children to reflect on the causes of their uncertainty about a claim did not directly increase the likelihood that they suggested an efficient test, our exploratory analyses showed that the ability to provide a plausible reason for their beliefs did—even when controlling for their ability to reason scientifically. This provides support for the notion that developments in children’s reasoning about their beliefs drive changes in their empirical evaluation of those beliefs.</p> <p>&nbsp;</p>
You should fill this box only if you chose 'All or part of the results presented in this preprint are based on data'. URL must start with http:// or https://
You should fill this box only if you chose 'Scripts were used to obtain or analyze the results'. URL must start with http:// or https://
You should fill this box only if you chose 'Codes have been used in this study'. URL must start with http:// or https://
Information seeking, Uncertainty reasoning, Testing claims, Exploration
NonePlease indicate the methods that may require specialised expertise during the peer review process (use a comma to separate various required expertises).
Social sciences
Amy Masnick suggested: Christopher Osterhaus christopher.osterhaus@uni-vechta.de , Amy Masnick suggested: Susanne Koerber susanne.koerber@ph-freiburg.de
e.g. John Doe john@doe.com
No need for them to be recommenders of PCI Registered Reports. Please do not suggest reviewers for whom there might be a conflict of interest. Reviewers are not allowed to review preprints written by close colleagues (with whom they have published in the last four years, with whom they have received joint funding in the last four years, or with whom they are currently writing a manuscript, or submitting a grant proposal), or by family members, friends, or anyone for whom bias might affect the nature of the review - see the code of conduct
e.g. John Doe john@doe.com
2024-06-19 09:39:15
Chris Chambers