
Are there oscillatory markers of pain intensity?

The effect of stimulus saliency on the modulation of ongoing neural oscillations related to thermonociception: a Registered Report
Abstract
Recommendation: posted 20 February 2025, validated 20 February 2025
Dienes, Z. (2025) Are there oscillatory markers of pain intensity?. Peer Community in Registered Reports, 100940. 10.24072/pci.rr.100940
This is a stage 2 based on:
Chiara Leu, Sébastien Forest, Valéry Legrain, Giulia Liberati
https://osf.io/c9b4p
Recommendation
Level of bias control achieved: Level 6. No part of the data or evidence that was used to answer the research question was generated until after IPA.
- Advances in Cognitive Psychology
- Brain and Neuroscience Advances
- Cortex
- F1000Research
- Imaging Neuroscience
- In&Vertebrates
- NeuroImage: Reports
- Peer Community Journal
- PeerJ
- Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research and Practice
- Royal Society Open Science
- Studia Psychologica
1. Leu, C., Forest, S., Legrain, V., & Liberati, G. (2025). The effect of stimulus saliency on the modulation of pain-related ongoing neural oscillations: a Registered Report [Stage 2]. Acceptance of Version 3 by Peer Community in Registered Reports. https://osf.io/98edq
The recommender in charge of the evaluation of the article and the reviewers declared that they have no conflict of interest (as defined in the code of conduct of PCI) with the authors or with the content of the article.
Evaluation round #2
DOI or URL of the report: https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/S3879
Version of the report: 2
Author's Reply, 18 Feb 2025
Decision by Zoltan Dienes
, posted 17 Feb 2025, validated 18 Feb 2025
Thank you for your revisions. I have just a couple of points:
1) p 6 " Due to non-compliance or artifacted signals, data of some participants were discarded from the analyses." Be clear if these exclusions were based on pre-registered criteria.
2) When a hypothesis predicts an effect, and the power was not there to pick up all effects that were in principle large enough to support the prediction, then a non-significant result does not disconfirm the hypothesis. It leaves it open. Be more clear about this when discussing non-signfiicant results.
best
Zoltan
Evaluation round #1
DOI or URL of the report: https://osf.io/6fcge
Version of the report: 1
Author's Reply, 03 Feb 2025
Decision by Zoltan Dienes
, posted 28 Dec 2024, validated 30 Dec 2024
The two reviewers are largely positive about your Stage 2, though make some excellent points. Reviewer 1 urges structuring more closely based on the Study Design Table; indeed, as I was reading the Results section, I had to have the Study Design Table up in another window to go through step by step to check, and it still needed a fair amount of cognitive effort. You could go through listing each question given in the Table, and then the relevant test. Likewise the Discussion, as suggested by the Reviewer. Reviewer 2 raises some relevant substantive points to consider.
On the subject of the Table, note how non-significance was declared as not indicating no effect. As the literature almost universally rides roughshod over this point, it can be hard to break habits absorbed from the literature! Go through thoroughly making sure you do not claim there are no effects when there was simply non-significance. For example: p 24 "while no difference was found between oddball and baseline cycles in the low oddball condition stimulation (F(750)=0.0404, p=0.841)"
"no difference" -> "no significant difference"(as you have not established there was no difference.) Same point arises on page 30, 33, 34, 36, 37. There may be other places.
For all changes to IPA, note them in a footnote at the point they are first mentioned, together with when the change was approved by PCI RR.
best
Zoltan
Reviewed by Markus Ploner
, 04 Dec 2024
The stage 2 manuscript mostly follows the outlines of the stage 1 manuscript. Different than initially planned, the stimulation hardly elicited painful sensations. Therefore, the title and the abstract have been changed to account for this lack of pain induction. Beyond that, the main finding is that high oddball stimuli elicited painful sensations and neural responses, which differed from baseline stimuli. In contrast, the low oddball stimuli did not. Thus, no definite conclusion can be drawn regarding the relationship between stimulus saliency, stimulus intensity, pain perception, and ongoing oscillations.
The manuscript presents the results mostly clearly and discusses the findings extensively. Some revisions might further improve the manuscript:
1. Title and abstract. Unlike initially planned, the stimulation hardly elicited painful sensations, which changed the title and the abstract from pain perception to thermonociception. I’m wondering whether this tacit focus change is in accordance with the idea of a registered report. The authors might consider presenting and explaining this change in the abstract.
2. Results. The presentation of the results does not clearly relate them to the hypothesis table. I understand the hypothesis table as a core part of a registered report that should guide the analysis and interpretation. Thus, the authors should explicitly relate the findings to the hypothesis table.
3. Discussion. The discussion is quite long and discusses many details of the paradigm, but the results and their interpretation are less discussed along the hypothesis table. The authors might significantly shorten the discussion and change the focus from discussing methodological details to the main topic and the interpretation of the main results.
Reviewed by Bjoern Horing
, 22 Dec 2024
Let me first say that I am excited that the results are in! Thank you for a well thought-out and informative study, it’s been a pleasure accompanying this process.
In the following, page numbers refer to the clean PDF. All comments are rather minor, but I would strongly argue for a re-reading of the discussion that seems a bit frayed occasionally.
GENERAL COMMENTS
- The abstract does not mention the (final) sample size which seems like a relevant information for me; furthermore, it is not clear from the descriptions in 2.1 versus 3. whether the final sample size is 35 or 33 (for behavioral data) and 31 (for EEG data), respectively - or if the sample was expanded considering the drop-outs
- 3.1, paragraph 1: The authors suggest to "see Supplementary Material for single subject average examples"; later it more specifically points to "see Supplementary Materials S.IV for single subject average examples"; however, that section seems not to exist, at least not in the two PDFs provided (RR_Saliency Stage II_clean, RR_Saliency Stage II_marked)
- 3.1, paragraph 2: It is quite surprising that the (ostensibly) high temperatures employed did not yield robust pain; it would be an interesting data point exactly what these temperatures were, so could the authors provide at least the descriptives (calibrated mean±SD), at best a plot showing perception threshold, pain threshold, and max temperature (possibly akin to Fig 5)? This may seem pedantic at this point, but I feel it’s pertinent given that the thermal stimulation seems to have been one of the big issues during actual empiry.
- 3.3.2, line 7, “considering the first two harmonics in the high oddball condition” is unfortunately phrased, you mean “including the first two harmonics…” (in addition to the oddball frequency itself), right? So FOSagg is 0.125+0.25+0.375, not just 0.25+0.375. This confusion also exists on p. 35, line 2 (it’s activity at the first 2 harmonics AND the oddball frequency).
- 4.1, the high oddball supposedly was “the only stimulation that was consistently perceived as painful” seems to be a stretch: Fig 5 and the top mean±SD of 5.5±2.4 VAS (5 being pain threshold) clearly indicate that large portions of the sample did not in fact perceive them as painful. I suggest replacing “consistently” with “on average”.
- Parts of the discussion have a rushed feel and should be revisited, e.g.
- p. 32 line 7f., “known to be the primarily” => do you mean primary contributors/afferents/some such notion?
- p. 32 I would think it’s a “heat sink-effect”, not a “heat sink”? Also use “larger” instead of “worse”?
- p. 32 remove the “, which” after “Wang et al. (2022)”
- p. 34 “… following the oddball stimulation in the low oddball condition”, the wording here and in the following is a bit ambiguous and you shift from referencing high oddball/low oddball/unspecified oddball results; can you revisit this?
- All heat sink-related or neuronal temporal filtering aside (enjoyable as the discussion is), it seems to me that another explanation of high oddball-salience and the absence of low oddball-salience is simply that only the high oddball recruited nociceptors to begin with. Hypothetically, assuming an absence of pain perception for the baseline stimulus (VAS 4.6/VAS 5.0 depending on calculation) chiefly due to an absence of nociceptive drive, the high oddball-related temperature increase might have pushed the stimulus above the pain threshold, leading to a discrete and very salient percept (as in, a new sensory modality arises), whereas baseline/low oddball fluctuations all remain within the non-noxious heat range. This interpretation is tempered, of course, by the fact that roughly half the sample would have actually perceived the baseline peaks as painful (>5.0), as well, without modality-related salience of the high oddball.
- Another alternative interpretation the authors may or may not want to explore is an accumulating offset effects from the downward slopes, maybe fostering a larger antinociceptive tone of the descending modulation (they already point to the comparatively short time spent "at peak", by nature of the stimulation).
MINOR FORMAL ISSUES
- p. 23 has at least 2 FOSoddball subscript issues
- p. 29 line 5 subscript period [sub].[/sub]
- p. 31 probably => probable