Submit a report

Announcements

=============================================================================

IMPORTANT ANNOUNCEMENT: To accommodate reviewer and recommender holiday schedules, we will be closed to ALL submissions from 1st Jul - 1st Sep. During this time, reviewers can submit reviews and recommenders can issue decisions, but no new or revised submissions can be made by authors.

The one exception to this rule is that authors using the scheduled track who submit their initial Stage 1 snapshot prior to 1st Jul can choose a date within the shutdown period to submit their full Stage 1 manuscript.

We recommend that authors submit at least 1-2 weeks prior to commencement of the shutdown period to enable time to make any required revisions prior to in-depth review.

=============================================================================

We are recruiting recommenders (editors) from all research fields!

Your feedback matters! If you have authored or reviewed a Registered Report at Peer Community in Registered Reports, then please take 5 minutes to leave anonymous feedback about your experience, and view community ratings.

Latest recommendations

IdTitle * Authors * Abstract * PictureThematic fields * RecommenderReviewersSubmission date
20 May 2025
STAGE 1

Revisiting celebrity contagion and the value of objects: Replication and extensions Registered Report of Newman et al. (2011)

Does a "contagion effect" explain high valuation of celebrity items?

Recommended by ORCID_LOGO based on reviews by Miguel Vadillo, Saleh Shuqair, Lachlan Deer and Susanne Adler
In 2024, a shirt worn by renowned baseball player Babe Ruth during the 1932 World Series sold for a record-breaking $24.1m, making it one of the most expensive collector’s items ever sold at auction. Ruth’s famous “called shot” jersey is just one of countless items previously owned by celebrities that routinely fetch eye-watering prices despite having no obvious use. Understanding what gives these objects their subjective value presents an intriguing challenge in economic and social psychology.
 
An influential study by Newman et al. (2011) proposed three explanations for such behaviour – first, that the object acts as a memento to remind the consumer of the celebrity; second, that the object is perceived to have a high market demand and is believed to attract a higher price on resale; and third that the object triggers a contagion effect – a phenomenon, observed in a variety of psychological contexts, in which the consumer believes that some intangible essence of the object’s previous owner might be transmitted to them through physical contact. Across a series of experiments, Newman et al. (2011) concluded that the evidence favours an explanation in terms of contagion.
 
Here, Chan et al. (2025) propose a large online study (N=1200; several times larger than the original study) to partially replicate Experiments 1 and 2 in Newman et al. (2011). The authors aim to establish the reliability of this supposed contagion effect and to test its potential dependence on proximity – that is, whether the desire to have physical contact with the person is necessary for the contagion effect to occur. Specifically, for their main questions they ask whether fame (celebrity, non-celebrity) and moral valence of the previous owner (positive, negative, mixed) influence item valuation, desire for physical contact, market demand, and willingness to purchase. To further test any dependence on proximity, the authors will ask whether fame and valence impact desire for non-physical contact.
 
The Stage 1 manuscript was evaluated over two rounds of in-depth review. Based on detailed responses to the reviewers' comments, the recommender judged that the manuscript met the Stage 1 criteria and therefore awarded in-principle acceptance (IPA).
 
URL to the preregistered Stage 1 protocol: https://osf.io/gnrhf
 
Level of bias control achieved: Level 6. No part of the data or evidence that will be used to answer the research question yet exists and no part will be generated until after IPA. 
 
List of eligible PCI RR-friendly journals:
 
 
References
 
1. Newman, G. E., Diesendruck, G., & Bloom, P. (2011). Celebrity contagion and the value of objects. Journal of Consumer Research, 38, 215–228. https://doi.org/10.1086/658999
 
2. Chan, M.,  Jin, Y., Chen, E. Y., Peng, S., Charlton, A. & Feldman, G. (2025). Revisiting celebrity contagion and the value of objects: Replication and extensions Registered Report of Newman et al. (2011). In principle acceptance of Version 3 by Peer Community in Registered Reports. https://osf.io/gnrhf
Revisiting celebrity contagion and the value of objects: Replication and extensions Registered Report of Newman et al. (2011)Mannix Chan, Yaqi Jin, Eva Yiyu Chen, Shanshan Peng, Aaron Charlton, Gilad Feldman<p>​[IMPORTANT: The abstract, method, and results sections were all written using a randomized dataset produced by Qualtrics to simulate what these sections will look like after data collection. These will be updated following the actual data coll...Social sciencesChris Chambers2024-05-23 15:21:31 View
20 May 2025
STAGE 2
(Go to stage 1)

Revisiting mental accounting classic paradigms: Replication of the problems reviewed in Thaler (1999)

Mental accounting under the microscope

Recommended by ORCID_LOGO based on reviews by Barnabas Szaszi and Féidhlim McGowan
In recent years the study of mental accounting – the thought processes by which people informally record, categorise, and evaluate the costs and benefits of their financial transactions – has been an active research area, drawing attention to a range of biases and distortions that deviate from optimal economic decision-making (Zhang & Sussman, 2018). Although the term “mental accounting” is a relatively recent construction (Thaler, 1999), it stems from a longer history of behavioural economic research on value functions, decision frames, risk-taking, and related concepts.
 
In the current study, Li and Feldman (2025) replicated 17 influential mental accounting problems (or tasks) reviewed by Thaler (1999) in a large online sample. The authors also undertook several extensions examining the effects of sunk costs and expenses framing.
 
The results suggest that, broadly speaking, mental accounting phenomena replicate successfully, but with greater complexity and contextual dependency than implied by previous work. Of the problems examined, the authors found empirical support for 11, mixed support for 3, and no support for 3. The extended questions returned similarly partial support.
 
The Stage 2 manuscript was evaluated over one round of in-depth review. Based on detailed responses to the reviewers' comments, the recommender judged that the manuscript met the Stage 2 criteria and awarded a positive recommendation.
 
URL to the preregistered Stage 1 protocol: https://osf.io/d6cjk
 
Level of bias control achieved: Level 6. No part of the data or evidence that was used to answer the research question was generated until after IPA. 
 
List of eligible PCI RR-friendly journals:
 
References
 
1. Zhang, C. Y., & Sussman, A. B. (2018). Perspectives on mental accounting: An exploration of budgeting and investing. Financial Planning Review, 1, e1011. https://doi.org/10.1002/cfp2.1011
 
2. Thaler, R. H. (1999). Mental accounting matters. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 12, 183-206. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0771(199909)12:3%3C183::AID-BDM318%3E3.0.CO;2-F
 
3. Li, M. & Feldman, G. (2022). Revisiting mental accounting classic paradigms: Replication of the problems reviewed in Thaler (1999) [Stage 2]. Acceptance of Version 4 by Peer Community in Registered Reports. https://osf.io/pkw4g
Revisiting mental accounting classic paradigms: Replication of the problems reviewed in Thaler (1999)Mengfei Li; Gilad Feldman<p>Mental accounting, the internal categorization system individuals adopt to manage their financial activities, may result in suboptimal decisions or decision-making not aligned with one’s own goals. In a Registered Report with an online U.S. sam...Social sciencesChris Chambers2023-01-15 13:29:22 View
20 May 2025
STAGE 2
(Go to stage 1)

No facilitatory effects of transcranial random noise stimulation on motion processing: A registered report

No evidence that transcranial random noise stimulation influences motion processing

Recommended by ORCID_LOGO based on reviews by Samuel Westwood
High frequency transcranial random noise stimulation (hf-tRNS) is a relatively new form of non-invasive brain stimulation thought to enhance neural excitability and facilitate processing in targeted brain areas. The evidence for the efficacy of hf-tRNS is mixed, so a high-powered test of the proposed facilitatory effects is of high value to the field.
 
In the current study, Edwards et al. (2025) targeted the human middle temporal complex (hMT+), an area with a well-established critical role in global motion processing. The protocol was adapted from a study by Ghin et al. (2018) and focused on a sub-set of the original experimental conditions using a fully within-subjects design (n=42). Global motion processing was operationalised in terms of the coherence threshold for identification of the dominant direction of random-dot motion.
 
The experiment tested the predicted facilitation of contralateral motion processing (reduced coherence threshold) during hf-tRNS to the left hMT+. In particular, the specificity of this effect was tested by comparison to a sham stimulation control condition and an active stimulation control condition (left forehead). By targeting a brain area with a well-established critical role in behaviour, the authors aimed to evaluate the replicability and specificity of the facilitatory effects of hf-tRNS.
 
The results provided no evidence that hf-tRNS improves motion discrimination, with no significant facilitation of contralateral global motion processing following hf-tRNS to hM+, and no significant difference between hf-tRNS to hMT+ in comparison to either sham stimulation or forehead stimulation. These findings question the reliability and generalisability of tRNS as a neurocognitive intervention and call for a coordinated programme of high-powered research to establish the parameters under which such effects arise – ideally using the Registered Reports format to eliminate reporting bias.
 
Following one round of in-depth review, the recommender judged that the manuscript met the Stage 2 criteria and awarded a positive recommendation.
 
URL to the preregistered Stage 1 protocol: https://osf.io/bce7u
 
Level of bias control achieved: Level 6. No part of the data or evidence that was used to answer the research question was generated until after IPA. 
 
List of eligible PCI RR-friendly journals:
 
References
 
1. Ghin, F., Pavan, A., Contillo, A., & Mather, G. (2018). The effects of high-frequency transcranial random noise stimulation (hf-tRNS) on global motion processing: an equivalent noise approach. Brain Stimulation, 11, 1263–75.
 
2. Edwards, G., Ruhde, R., Carroll, M. B., & Baker, C. I. (2025). No facilitatory effects of transcranial random noise stimulation on motion processing: A registered report [Stage 2]. Acceptance of Version 4 by Peer Community in Registered Reports. https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2025.03.18.643903v4.full.pdf
No facilitatory effects of transcranial random noise stimulation on motion processing: A registered reportGrace Edwards, Ryan Ruhde, Mica B. Carroll, Chris I. Baker<p>Non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) techniques have the potential to demonstrate the causal impact of targeted brain regions on specific behaviors, and to regulate or facilitate behavior in clinical applications. Transcranial random noise sti...Life SciencesChris Chambers2025-03-19 15:07:36 View
20 May 2025
STAGE 2
(Go to stage 1)

See me, judge me, pay me: Gendered effort moralization in work and care

A gender difference in effort moralization?

Recommended by ORCID_LOGO based on reviews by Jared Celniker
Effort moralization is the well-known idea that, unrelated to actual performance, people making more effort are judged better, attributed more morality and seen as better collaborators than people making less effort. However, previous studies on this topic mostly used vignettes with a man or a neutral protagonist. The current study by Roth et al. (2025) tackled the gender problem by testing the difference in attribution morality between a man and a woman protagonist, and two contexts: a “care” and a “work” context, mirroring the stereotypes associated with men and women.
 
The authors included two different and adequate power analyses, various interpretation of the possible effects, and filtering to ensure a high quality of data collection. They also provide a supplementary repository including the qualtrics survey, R script, and simulated data.
 
By using a conceptual replication of a study by Celniker et al. (2023), the results indicated strong support for a generalization of the effort moralization theory in a work context. However, for the "care" scenario, the result was mixed, and might be due to the moral evaluation of the effort. Results were more surprising for the gender effect, as the results did not indicate difference by gender, and no interaction between gender and scenario. Therefore, the effect could be related to a work context that does not involve morality and seems to be generalizable across gender.
 
The Stage 2 manuscript was evaluated over two rounds of in-depth review. Based on ​detailed responses to the reviewers’ and the recommender’s comments, the recommender judged that the manuscript met the Stage 2 criteria and awarded a positive recommendation.

URL to the preregistered Stage 1 protocol: https://osf.io/xd87r
 
Level of bias control achieved: Level 6. No part of the data or evidence that was used to answer the research question was generated until after IPA.
 
List of eligible PCI RR-friendly journals:
 
 
References
 
Celniker, J. B., Gregory, A., Koo, H. J., Piff, P. K., Ditto, P. H., & Shariff, A. F. (2023). The moralization of effort. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 152, 60–79. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001259
 
Roth, L. H. O., Tissot, T. T., Fischer, T. & Masak, S. C. (2025). See me, judge me, pay me: Gendered effort moralization in work and care [Stage 2]. Acceptance of Version 3 by Peer Community in Registered Reports. https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/wkbs6_v3
See me, judge me, pay me: Gendered effort moralization in work and careLeopold H. O. Roth, Tassilo T. Tissot, Thea Fischer, S. Charlotte Masak<p>Displaying high effort at work is rewarded with more positive moral judgments (effort moralization effect) and increased cooperation partner attractiveness. This holds, even if higher effort is unrelated to better performance. Yet, current evid...Social sciencesAdrien Fillon2025-03-03 15:31:20 View
19 May 2025
STAGE 1

Revisiting the morality of vegetarianism and veganism: Replication Registered Report of Rozin et al. (1997)

Replicating the moralization of vegetarianism

Recommended by ORCID_LOGO based on reviews by Ben De Groeve, Willem Sleegers and Seth Green
Psychology needs more replication. For the field to be a cumulative and robust science, we need to be able to verify that effects hold up in new samples. Particularly in the case of seminal findings that inspired long lines of follow-up research, it is of interest to assess if the original result is reliable.
 
In the current study, Leong and colleagues (2025) propose a high-powered direct replication of a seminal experiment by Rozin et al. (1997). In an online sample, the authors investigate attitudinal differences between moral vegetarians and health vegetarians, to study the consequences of moralization. The authors stay close to the original design, in order to allow for a clear conclusion on whether the findings of the original study has been successfully replicated or not. This study is part of a large collaborative effort to replicate seminal findings in in social psychology and decision-making.
 
This replication study has the potential to 1) increase our understanding of moralization, especially in the context of vegetarianism, and 2) allow for a systematic assessment of replicability in the social sciences.
 
The Stage 1 manuscript was evaluated over two rounds of in-depth review. Based on detailed responses to the reviewers' comments, the recommender judged that the manuscript met the Stage 1 criteria and therefore awarded in-principle acceptance (IPA).
 
URL to the preregistered Stage 1 protocol: https://osf.io/6pyhf
 
Level of bias control achieved: Level 6. No part of the data or evidence that will be used to answer the research question yet exists and no part will be generated until after IPA.
 
List of eligible PCI RR-friendly journals:
 
 
References
 
1. Leong, U., Chan, E. H. L., Yau, W. T., Ng, W. Y., Chim, T. W., Yeung, W., Tam, K. Y. Y., & Feldman, G. (2025). Revisiting the morality of vegetarianism and veganism: Replication Registered Report of Rozin et al. (1997). In principle acceptance of Version 3 by Peer Community in Registered Reports. https://osf.io/6pyhf
 
2. Rozin, P., Markwith, M., & Stoess, C. (1997). Moralization and becoming a vegetarian: The transformation of preferences into values and the recruitment of disgust. Psychological Science, 8, 67-73. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00685.x
Revisiting the morality of vegetarianism and veganism: Replication Registered Report of Rozin et al. (1997)Utek Leong, Esther Hiu Lam Chan, Wing Tung Yau, Wing Yan Ng, Tsz Wah Chim, Wing Yeung, Katy Y. Y. Tam, Gilad Feldman<p>[IMPORTANT: Abstract, method, and results were written using a randomized dataset produced by Qualtrics to simulate what these sections will look like after data collection. These will be updated following the data collection. For the purpose o...Social sciencesMichèle Nuijten2024-09-08 05:49:21 View
19 May 2025
STAGE 2
(Go to stage 1)
article picture

A multilab investigation into the N2pc as an indicator of attentional selectivity: Direct replication of Eimer (1996)

Is the N2pc a correlate of attentional selection? An #EEGManyLabs multi-lab registered replication of Eimer (1996)

Recommended by ORCID_LOGO based on reviews by Reny Baykova and Clayton Hickey
In attention research, the N2pc ERP – a lateralised negative deflection over parieto-occipital electrodes – is often interpreted as a marker of attentional selection, so much so that it is frequently used a tool in the attention literature for inferring that a stimulus was attentionally processed. This interpretation of N2pc has its roots in the seminal work of Eimer (1996), wherein the N2pc was observed when participants performed an attentional selection task with either colour or form (letter) stimuli. Despite its enormous influence in attention research, this work has never been directly replicated.
 
Here, Constant et al. (2025) conducted a high-powered replication attempt of the critical Study 2 of Eimer (1996), as part of the #EEGManyLabs (Pavlov et al., 2021) project. Twenty two labs across 14 countries took part and the N2pc was tested for using four pre-registered and one exploratory pipelines.
 
Results showed that the N2pc for form stimuli was remarkably robust, replicated by every participating lab under all four pre-registered (and one exploratory) preprocessing pipelines. By contrast, the N2pc for colour stimuli did not technically replicate, though a negative deflection was found 70ms earlier than originally reported.
 
This is a gold-standard replication attempt that should be an invaluable resource to the selective attention field.
 
The Stage 2 manuscript was evaluated over one round of in-depth review by two reviewers who also reviewed the Stage 1 report. Based on detailed responses to the reviewers' comments, the recommender judged that the manuscript met the Stage 2 criteria and awarded a positive recommendation.
 
URL to the preregistered Stage 1 protocol: https://osf.io/dw68r
 
Level of bias control achieved: Level 6. No part of the data or evidence that was used to answer the research question was generated until after IPA.
 
List of eligible PCI RR-friendly journals:
 
 
References
 
1. Constant, M., Mandal, A., Asanowicz, D., Panek, B., Kotlewska, I., Yamaguchi, M., Gillmeister, H., Kerzel, D., Luque, D., Molinero, S., Vázquez-Millán, A., Pesciarelli, F., Borelli, E., Ramzaoui, H., Beck, M., Somon, B., Desantis, A., Castellanos, M. C., Martín-Arévalo, E., Manini, G., Capizzi, M., Gokce, A., Özer, D., Soyman, E., Yılmaz, E., Eayrs, J. O., London, R. E., Steendam, T., Frings, C., Pastötter, B., Szaszkó, B., Baess, P., Ayatollahi, S., León Montoya, G. A., Wetzel, N., Widmann, A., Cao, L., Low, X., Costa, T. L., Chelazzi, L., Monachesi, B., Kamp, S.-M., Knopf, L., Itier, R. J., Meixner, J., Jost, K., Botes, A., Braddock, C., Li, D., Nowacka, A., Quenault, M., Scanzi, D., Torrance, T., Corballis, P. M., Laera, G., Kliegel, M., Welke, D., Mushtaq, F., Pavlov, Y. G., & Liesefeld, H. R. (2025). A multilab investigation into the N2pc as an indicator of attentional selectivity: Direct replication of Eimer (1996) [Stage 2]. Acceptance of Version 3 by Peer Community in Registered Reports. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/3472y_v3
 
2. Eimer, M. (1996). The N2pc component as an indicator of attentional selectivity. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 99, 225-234. https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(96)95711-9
 
3. Pavlov, Y. G., Adamian, N., Appelhoff, S., Arvaneh, M., Benwell, C. S. Y., Beste, C., Bland, A. R., Bradford, D. E., Bublatzky, F., Busch, N. A., Clayson, P. E., Cruse, D., Czeszumski, A., Dreber, A., Dumas, G., Ehinger, B., Ganis, G., He, X., Hinojosa, J. A., Huber-Huber, C., Inzlicht, M., Jack, B. N., Johannesson, M., Jones, R., Kalenkovich, E., Kaltwasser, L., Karimi-Rouzbahani, H., Keil, A., König, P., Kouara, L., Kulke, L., Ladouceur, C. D., Langer, N., Liesefeld, H. R., Luque, D., MacNamara, A., Mudrik, L., Muthuraman, M., Neal, L. B., Nilsonne, G., Niso, G., Ocklenburg, S., Oostenveld, R., Pernet, C. R., Pourtois, G., Ruzzoli, M., Sass, S. M., Schaefer, A., Senderecka, M., Snyder, J. S., Tamnes, C. K., Tognoli, E., van Vugt, M. K., Verona, E., Vloeberghs, R., Welke, D., Wessel, J. R., Zakharov, I., & Mushtaq, F. (2021). #EEGManyLabs: Investigating the replicability of influential EEG experiments. Cortex, 144, 213-229. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2021.03.013
A multilab investigation into the N2pc as an indicator of attentional selectivity: Direct replication of Eimer (1996)Martin Constant, Ananya Mandal, Dariusz Asanowicz, Bartłomiej Panek, Ilona Kotlewska, Motonori Yamaguchi, Helge Gillmeister, Dirk Kerzel, David Luque, Sara Molinero, Antonio Vázquez-Millán, Francesca Pesciarelli, Eleonora Borelli, Hanane Ramzaoui,...<p>The N2pc is widely employed as an electrophysiological marker of an attention allocation. This interpretation was largely driven by the observation of an N2pc elicited by an isolated relevant target object, which was reported as Experiment 2 in...Social sciencesMaxine Sherman2025-02-14 17:38:12 View
19 May 2025
STAGE 2
(Go to stage 1)

The Harmful Dysfunction Analysis applied to the concept of behavioral addiction: A secondary analysis of data from the Health Behaviour in School-aged Children 2018

Applying Harmful Dysfunction Analysis to social media usage in adolescents

Recommended by based on reviews by Veli-Matti Karhulahti, Gemma Lucy Smart and Josip Razum
Amendola and colleagues (2025) examined the usefulness of Harmful Dysfunction Analysis (HDA) in identifying individuals with pathological social media disorder. Harmful Dysfunction Analysis, proposed by Wakefield et al (1992), is a framework for constructing diagnostic criteria, and suggests a disorder is a harmful dysfunction, and defines a dysfunction as a failure of an internal mechanism to perform its naturally designed function. One important distinction of HDA is that harmful consequences of behaviour in the absence of dysfunction does not mean a disorder is present. Both dysfunction and harm are required for diagnosis of a disorder
 
Their analysis is based on data from the Health Behaviour in School-aged Children study (specifically the Swiss sample, N = 7,510), which is a world health organisation collaborative cross-sectional study of adolescent well-being from 2018 (https://www.who.int/europe/initiatives/health-behaviour-in-school-aged-children-(hbsc)-study). They examined the 9-item Social Media Disorder Scale under DSM-5 and HDA categories (dysfunction and harm), and examine convergence between the scoring methods. 
 
Their findings suggested higher prevalence of problematic social media usage and gaming disorder prevalence (33.2 and 9%, respectively) using substance use disorder (DSM-5) compared to prevalence rates of 22.2% and 4.2% when using Harmful Dysfunction analyses. Sensitivity analyses demonstrated similarly findings in a sample of individuals from Hungary. They suggest that their findings might help to eliminate false positive diagnoses for individuals who engage in high intensity social media / gaming usage, that might be mistaken for a mental disorder.  However, as discussed by the authors caution is warranted findings are limited to adolescents (aged 11-15) and based on self-reported variables. 
 
The Stage 2 manuscript was evaluated by two expert reviewers across one round of in-depth review. Based on detailed responses to the reviewers' comments, the recommender judged that the manuscript met the Stage 2 criteria and awarded a positive recommendation.
 
URL to the preregistered Stage 1 protocol: https://osf.io/y3ub8
 
Level of bias control achieved: Level 3. At least some data/evidence that was used to the answer the research question had been previously accessed by the authors (e.g. downloaded or otherwise received), but the authors certify that they had not observed any part of the data/evidence prior Stage 1 to in-principle acceptance.
 
List of eligible PCI RR-friendly journals:
 
 
References
 
1. Amendola, S., Hengartner, M. P., & Wakefield, J. C. (2025). TThe Harmful Dysfunction Analysis applied to the concept of behavioral addiction: A secondary analysis of data from the Health Behaviour in School-aged Children 2018 [Stage 2]. Acceptance of Version 3 by Peer Community in Registered Reports. https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/atwgy_v3
 
2. Wakefield, J. C. (1992). Disorder as Harmful Dysfunction: A Conceptual Critique of DSM-III-R’s Definition of Mental Disorder. Psychological Review, 99, 232–247. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0033-295X.99.2.232
 
The Harmful Dysfunction Analysis applied to the concept of behavioral addiction: A secondary analysis of data from the Health Behaviour in School-aged Children 2018Simone Amendola, Michael P. Hengartner, Jerome C. Wakefield<p>Objective: The study's principal aim was to explore the usefulness of the Harmful Dysfunction Analysis (HDA) in identifying individuals with pathological social media use (PSMU) and potentially other behavioral addictions as an alternative to u...Social sciencesAndrew Jones2025-01-13 22:51:20 View
12 May 2025
STAGE 2
(Go to stage 1)

Revisiting the link between true-self and morality: Replication and extension of Newman, Bloom, and Knobe (2014) Studies 1 and 2

Successfully replicating positive evaluations of our "true selves"

Recommended by ORCID_LOGO based on reviews by Andrew Christy, Cillian McHugh, Caleb Reynolds and Sergio Barbosa
The concept of a “true self” – the deepest and most genuine part of a person’s personality – is fundamental to many aspects of psychology, with influences that extend deep into society and culture. For decades, research in psychology has consistently found that people see their true selves as positive and virtuous. But people also positively regard (and indeed overestimate) many other characteristics related to the self, such as their abilities and achievements, prompting the question of whether there is anything special about the “true self” as a psychological concept. In an influential study, Newman et al. (2014) found that people were more likely to attribute morally good than morally bad changes in the behaviour of other people to their true selves. Crucially, they also found that our tendency to view the true self positively is shaped by our own moral values – in essence, what we regard as morally or politically good, we see in the true selves of others. Newman et al’s findings suggest that the tendency for us to regard our true self in a positive light stems from the specific nature of true self as a concept. 
 
In the current study, Lee and Feldman (2025) replicated two key studies from Newman et al. (2014) in a large online sample. In particular, they asked whether true-self attributions are higher for changes in behaviour that are morally positive compared to morally negative or neutral, and, further, how true-self attributions are aligned with personal moral/political views. The results confirmed the original findings: morally positive changes in others were perceived as more reflective of the true self than morally negative or neutral changes, and changes that were more aligned with participants' moral/political views were perceived as more reflective of the true self (regardless of whether liberal or conservative). Additional exploratory analyses revealed that social norms were positively associated with true self attributions. Overall, the outcomes constitute a successful replication of the original findings, adding weight to the conclusion that behaviours considered more aligned with moral values are perceived as more strongly reflecting a person’s “true self”.
 
The Stage 2 manuscript was evaluated over one round of in-depth review. Based on detailed responses to the reviewers' comments, the recommender judged that the manuscript met the Stage 2 criteria and awarded a positive recommendation.
 
URL to the preregistered Stage 1 protocol: https://osf.io/v2tpf
 
Level of bias control achieved: Level 6. No part of the data or evidence that was used to answer the research question was generated until after IPA. 
 
List of eligible PCI RR-friendly journals:
 
References
 
1. Newman, G. E., Bloom, P., & Knobe, J. (2014). Value judgments and the true self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 203–216. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167213508791
 
2. Lee, S. C. & Feldman, G. (2025). Revisiting the link between true-self and morality: Replication and extension Registered Report of Newman, Bloom, and Knobe (2014) Studies 1 and 2 [Stage 2]. Acceptance of Version 4 by Peer Community in Registered Reports. https://osf.io/zer3d
Revisiting the link between true-self and morality: Replication and extension of Newman, Bloom, and Knobe (2014) Studies 1 and 2Shuk Ching Lee, Gilad Feldman<p>Newman et al. (2014) demonstrated that behaviors that are more aligned with moral values are perceived as more strongly reflecting a person’s “true-self”, suggesting that morality plays an important role in how people perceive others’ essential...Social sciencesChris Chambers2023-01-12 02:52:09 View
12 May 2025
STAGE 1

Is CPP an ERP marker of evidence accumulation in perceptual decision-making? A multiverse study

Evaluating the role of the centro-parietal positivity in perceptual decision-making

Recommended by ORCID_LOGO based on reviews by April Shi Min Ching, Cassie Short and Caleb Stone
One of the hallmarks of adaptive behaviour is perceptual decision-making: the ability to select and integrate sensory inputs to guide judgments and actions. For decades, evidence accumulation models (EAMs) have been influential in shaping understanding of perceptual decision-making, proposing that evidence for different choices builds over time until a threshold is reached and a decision is triggered. At the same time, reliable biomarkers for evidence accumulation have been observed through single-unit recordings in non-human primates, particularly in parietal, frontal and premotor regions. In humans, the centro-parietal positivity (CPP) –  a positive deflection in the event-related potential (ERP) waveform – has emerged as a candidate proxy of perceptual decision-making, exhibiting accumulate-to-bound dynamics, modality-independence, and versatility to sensory inputs. However, much remains to be understood about the generalisability of the CPP across different behavioural contexts, from simple decision-making tasks (e.g. motion discrimination) to more complex judgments (e.g. emotion discrimination).
 
In the current study, Liu et al. (2025) will test the robustness of the CPP in human decision-making by leveraging secondary analysis of existing datasets. Broadly, they hypothesise that if the CPP is a reliable and generalisable biomarker then it should covary statistically with evidence accumulation, both at the trial level and across tasks that increase in complexity. To test this prediction, the authors will undertake joint modelling of behavioural and ERP data using drift-diffusion modelling, capturing variability within and between trials to estimate the relationship between the CPP and drift rate. In addition, the authors will use multiverse analysis to test the robustness of the observed relationships across a range of different analysis choices, with decision nodes focusing on the choice of CPP metric (build-up rate, amplitude, or peak amplitude) and pooling method in statistical analysis (trial-wise or bin-wise). Overall, the study promises to offer a fresh perspective – both methodologically and theoretically – on how CPP relates to perceptual decision-making.
 
The Stage 1 manuscript was evaluated over two rounds of in-depth review. Based on detailed responses to the reviewers' comments, the recommender judged that the manuscript met the Stage 1 criteria and therefore awarded in-principle acceptance (IPA).
 
URL to the preregistered Stage 1 protocol: https://osf.io/vwmzh
 
Level of bias control achieved: Level 3. At least some data/evidence that will be used to the answer the research question has been previously accessed by the authors (e.g. downloaded or otherwise received), but the authors certify that they have not yet observed any part of the data/evidence
 
List of eligible PCI RR-friendly journals:
 
 
References
 
Liu, Y., Yan, C., & Chuan-Peng, H. (2025). Is CPP an ERP marker of evidence accumulation in perceptual decision-making? A multiverse study. In principle acceptance of Version 4 by Peer Community in Registered Reports. https://osf.io/vwmzh
Is CPP an ERP marker of evidence accumulation in perceptual decision-making? A multiverse studyYikang Liu, Chuyao Yan, Hu Chuan-Peng<p>Perceptual decision-making is a crucial cognitive process where sensory information is integrated to form judgments and actions. This process is explained by evidence accumulation models (EAM), which posit that decision-makers gather and accumu...Life SciencesChris Chambers Caleb Stone, Cassie Short, April Shi Min Ching2024-09-03 18:27:35 View
08 May 2025
STAGE 2
(Go to stage 1)

Changes in memory function in adults following SARS-CoV-2 infection: findings from the Covid and Cognition online study

Evidence for General Long-Term Memory Impairment Following SARS-CoV-2 Infection

Recommended by ORCID_LOGO based on reviews by Phivos Phylactou, Mitul Mehta and 1 anonymous reviewer
COVID-19 has been associated with cognitive impairments, particularly in memory performance (Guo et al., 2022). Given that associative memory typically declines earlier than item memory in conditions such as mild cognitive impairment (Chen & Chang, 2016), it remains an open question whether COVID-19 differentially affects item and associative memory. Furthermore, it is unclear whether such effects are specific to verbal or non-verbal material. To investigate these questions, Weinerova et al. (2024) recruited participants via long-COVID Facebook groups and clinical settings. They reported a significant detrimental effect of COVID-19 status on long-term memory performance across tasks. However, they did not find evidence for their preregistered hypothesis of an interaction between COVID-19 status and either memory type (item vs. associative) or stimulus type (verbal vs. non-verbal).
 
In the Stage 1 report, the authors preregistered a Bayes Factor threshold of 6 as the criterion for supporting the alternative hypothesis. All planned analyses were preregistered, incorporating both frequentist methods (to replicate Guo et al., 2022) and Bayesian ANCOVA (to test the preregistered hypotheses). As data collection had been completed at the time of Stage 1 submission, we assigned a Level 3 bias control to the Stage 1 report.
 
In the Stage 2 report, the authors confirmed a general negative impact of COVID-19 on long-term memory accuracy. Using frequentist ANCOVA, they successfully replicated Guo et al. (2022)’s findings of reduced accuracy in verbal item and non-verbal associative memory among individuals with prior COVID-19 infection. However, they did not replicate the previously observed effect on reaction times. The Bayesian ANCOVA analyses did not reach the preregistered evidential threshold (BF > 6). Verbal associative memory, which was not part of the original replication attempt, also showed reduced accuracy in individuals with prior infection. In contrast, performance on non-verbal item memory tasks showed a ceiling effect, possibly due to methodological differences from Guo et al. (2022). Across all four memory tasks, the authors found robust evidence for decreased accuracy associated with COVID-19 status but no effect on reaction times.
 
Contrary to their hypotheses, models that included interactions between COVID-19 status and either memory type or stimulus type were less likely than the null model, suggesting a general, rather than specific, detrimental effect of COVID-19 on long-term memory.
 
Analyses of vaccination status yielded inconclusive results, likely due to limited sample size and uncertainty in participants' self-reports of vaccination timing relative to infection. The question of whether vaccination has protective effects on cognition remains unresolved and merits further investigation. The authors also reported tentative exploratory findings, such as a potential association between longer time since infection and slower reaction times in verbal item memory tasks. These observations require confirmation in future studies.
 
Overall, the study makes a valuable contribution by replicating prior findings and extending them to suggest a broad impairment of long-term memory associated with COVID-19.
 
The Stage 2 report was reviewed by the same three reviewers who had evaluated the Stage 1 submission. All reviewers agreed that the authors had addressed prior comments and had adhered to the preregistered methodology and analysis plan. As in the Stage 1 evaluation, we note the potential for selection bias introduced by recruiting participants via long-COVID Facebook groups. Individuals with post-infection cognitive complaints may have been more likely to volunteer, limiting generalizability to the broader SARS-CoV-2-infected population. This and other limitations are clearly acknowledged in the Discussion section of the Stage 2 manuscript. The recommender judged that the manuscript met all Stage 2 criteria for recommendation.
 
URL to the preregistered Stage 1 protocol: https://osf.io/tjs5u
 
Level of bias control achieved: Level 3. At least some data/evidence that was used to the answer the research question had been accessed by the authors prior to in-principle acceptance (e.g. downloaded or otherwise received), but the authors certify that they had not yet observed ANY part of the data/evidence.
 
List of eligible PCI RR-friendly journals:
 
References

1. Chen, P.-C., & Chang, Y.-L. (2016, May). Associative memory and underlying brain correlates in older adults with mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia, 85, 216–225. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.03.032
 
2. Guo, P., Benito Ballesteros, A., Yeung, S. P., Liu, R., Saha, A., Curtis, L., ... Cheke, L. G. (2022b). COVCOG 2: Cognitive and memory deficits in long COVID: A second publication from the COVID and Cognition Study. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 14, 804937. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2022.804937
 
3. Weinerova, J., Yeung, S., Guo, P., Yau, A., Horne, C., Ghinn, M., Curtis, L., Adlard, F., Bhagat, V., Zhang, S., Kaser, M., Bozic, M., Schluppeck, D., Reid, A., Tibon, R., & Cheke, L. (2025). Changes in memory function in adults following SARS-CoV-2 infection: Findings from the Covid and Cognition online study [Stage 2]. Acceptance of Version 1 by Peer Community in Registered Reports. https://osf.io/q5vu2?view_only=228165eb161d490b945ca019143ba98c
Changes in memory function in adults following SARS-CoV-2 infection: findings from the Covid and Cognition online studyJosefina Weinerova, Sabine Yeung, Panyuan Guo, Alice Yau, Connor Horne, Molly Ghinn, Lyn Curtis, Frances Adlard, Vidita Bhagat, Seraphina Zhang, Muzaffer Kaser, Mirjana Bozic, Denis Schluppeck, Andrew Reid, Roni Tibon, Lucy Cheke<p>SARS-CoV-2, the virus responsible for the Covid-19 pandemic, has been shown to have an impact on cognitive function, but the specific aspects of cognition that are affected remain unclear. In this Registered Report, we analysed cognitive data c...Life SciencesVishnu SreekumarAnonymous, Phivos Phylactou, Benedict Michael, Mitul Mehta2024-11-21 12:32:53 View