Submit a report

Announcements

Please note that we will be CLOSED to ALL SUBMISSIONS from 1 December 2024 through 12 January 2025 to give our recommenders and reviewers a holiday break.

We are recruiting recommenders (editors) from all research fields!

Your feedback matters! If you have authored or reviewed a Registered Report at Peer Community in Registered Reports, then please take 5 minutes to leave anonymous feedback about your experience, and view community ratings.

860

The capacity of response training to help resist the consumption of sugary drinksuse asterix (*) to get italics
Hugo Najberg, Malika Tapparel, Lucas SpiererPlease use the format "First name initials family name" as in "Marie S. Curie, Niels H. D. Bohr, Albert Einstein, John R. R. Tolkien, Donna T. Strickland"
2024
<p style="text-align: justify;">Food response training has been shown to reduce the reported value of palatable food items. These approaches may thus help to reduce unhealthy (over)consumption behaviors and its related diseases. Yet, whether and how training-induced devaluation effects translate into reductions in the target items (over)consumption remains unclear. We addressed this issue by testing whether a combined food Go/NoGo and cue-approach training targeting healthy participants’ favorite sugary drinks can improve how many days they resist drinking them with a double-blind randomized controlled trial. We found that the 100% mapping of motor inhibition with the target unhealthy sugary drink cues in the experimental group did not increase the number of successful days of diet compared to the 50% mapping in the control group (30.7 vs 29.8 days). One possible interpretation of this result is that the training induced an equivalent effect in both groups, a hypothesis supported by the finding for equivalent target item devaluation in both groups. Another possible interpretation is that the training only induced an effect on the few participants prone to fail the diet early, while we recruited mostly resourceful healthy population, as supported by a difference in dieting adherence found only in participants with early failures (18% failure in the experimental group vs. 28.2% in the control group at first quartile). The other planned contrasts did not confirm a correlation between the devaluation effect induced by the training and the number of successful days of diet (r = -0.05), and identified a weak correlation between the number of days of training and the number of successful days of diet (r = 0.22). We propose to conduct another study that includes a control training focused on non-food, i.e. without any mapping with food cue.</p>
You should fill this box only if you chose 'All or part of the results presented in this preprint are based on data'. URL must start with http:// or https://
You should fill this box only if you chose 'Scripts were used to obtain or analyze the results'. URL must start with http:// or https://
You should fill this box only if you chose 'Codes have been used in this study'. URL must start with http:// or https://
food response training, food Go/NoGo, diet, cognitive intervention, consumption behavior
NonePlease indicate the methods that may require specialised expertise during the peer review process (use a comma to separate various required expertises).
Medical Sciences, Social sciences
Matthias Aulbach suggested: Sercan Kahveci sercan.kahveci@plus.ac.at , Matthias Aulbach suggested: Hannah van Alebeek hannah.vanalebeek@plus.ac.at, Matthias Aulbach suggested: Sercan Kahveci: sercan.kahveci@plus.ac.at , Matthias Aulbach suggested: Lucy Porter: lucy.porter@ucl.ac.uk , Matthias Aulbach suggested: Natalia Lawrence: Natalia.Lawrence@exeter.ac.uk
e.g. John Doe john@doe.com
No need for them to be recommenders of PCI Registered Reports. Please do not suggest reviewers for whom there might be a conflict of interest. Reviewers are not allowed to review preprints written by close colleagues (with whom they have published in the last four years, with whom they have received joint funding in the last four years, or with whom they are currently writing a manuscript, or submitting a grant proposal), or by family members, friends, or anyone for whom bias might affect the nature of the review - see the code of conduct
e.g. John Doe john@doe.com
2024-06-26 11:41:16
Zhang Chen